The Undoing Project
Remarkable
Profound
Timeless

The Undoing Project A Friendship that Changed the World

Michael Lewis2016
Their names were Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky. They were different in every way. But they were both obsessed with the human mind - and both happened to be geniuses. Together, they would change the way we see the world. 'An enchanted collaboration ... During the final pages, I was blinking back tears' The New York Times 'My favourite writer full stop. Engages both heart and brain like no other' Daily Telegraph 'Brilliant, a wonderful book, a masterclass' Spectator 'Psychology's Lennon and McCartney ... Lewis is exactly the storyteller they deserve' Observer
Sign up to use

Reviews

Photo of Matt Eaves
Matt Eaves@eavesyy
5 stars
Jul 5, 2024

“Papers written for social science journals are not intended for public consumption.” is how Lewis starts the sources chapter, but the opposite is why I like Lewis so much. In taking a super complex set of texts, Lewis can so easily distil to a reader their salient points and key learnings. Lewis writes things for public consumption, and he does it so well.

Photo of Timeo Williams
Timeo Williams@timeowilliams
4 stars
Jun 5, 2024

The book was a great historical account of Amos and Daniel, and the relationship that cam forth as a result. It also gave us great insight into how a new field within psychology emerged and just how irrational the human mind is.

Photo of Eitan Hershkovitz
Eitan Hershkovitz@ehershkovitz
5 stars
Aug 10, 2023

This book focuses on what they did, but also why it was only them that could do it.

Photo of Lord Aragorn
Lord Aragorn@lordaragorn
4 stars
Feb 19, 2023

Excellent background to the evolution of Behavioral Economics and biographies of Kahneman and Tversky

Photo of Nelson Zagalo
Nelson Zagalo@nzagalo
5 stars
Sep 3, 2022

O novo livro de Michael Lewis, autor do enorme sucesso “Moneyball” (2003) passado a filme homónimo em 2011 com Brad Pitt, é uma montanha russa de emoções. Usando como tema de fundo a amizade entre dois cientistas que revolucionaram a psicologia, Lewis leva-nos a conhecer o duo, dando conta de toda a sua genialidade sem descurar todas as fragilidades humanas. As páginas viram-se por si porque Lewis conta como poucos sabem contar uma boa história. É verdade que embeleza, que temos de ir colocando algum sal nos heroísmos, sonhos e facilidades que tão a jeito se colocam para nos lançar nos turbilhões emocionais, mas isso faz parte da arte do storytelling. Lewis não é um historiador, não está à procura da certeza absoluta, nem da total evidência daquilo que diz, Lewis é um contador de histórias, e usa toda a sua arte para nos inebriar e interessar pelo mundo da investigação científica. [imagem] Amos Tverski e Daniel Kahneman Daniel Kahneman tem hoje 83 anos, fugiu, com a sua família de Paris, ao Holocausto e chegou a Israel em 1946. Licenciou-se, com um major em Psicologia e um minor em Matemática, praticou psicologia e aprendeu a arte da investigação nas forças armadas israelitas. Nos anos 1960 iniciou o seu trabalho científico de fundo com um outro psicólogo matemático, Amos Tverski. Juntos, de Israel à Ivy League americana, transformariam a Psicologia e em consequência a Economia, levando ao desenvolvimento de uma área científica totalmente nova, a Economia Comportamental. Tverski morria de cancro em 1996 deixando Kahneman sozinho para receber o Prémio Nobel de Economia em 2002. Esta é a história que nos conta Michael Lewis, e que pelos ingredientes facilmente se poderá depreender que não faltam conflitos, medos e alegrias para criar interesse na leitura. Apesar de acreditar no livro como um excelente relato de proezas científicas, preenchido por uma boa componente humana que lhe confere grande empatia, recomendaria a qualquer leitor, se quiser extrair o máximo desta leitura, a ler primeiro “Pensar, Depressa e Devagar” (2011). Este é o livro que Daniel Kahneman e Amos Tverski tinham decidido escrever juntos, mas só acabaria por acontecer já depois da morte de Tverski e depois do Nobel. É um livro de divulgação científica, que abre o conhecimento complexo à leitura de leigos. E é um livro que não tenho parado de recomendar e recomendar a todos, porque é um livro que muda a forma como vemos o mundo, desde logo como nos vemos a nós mesmos. Daí que compreendendo melhor o alcance do trabalho de Kahneman e Tverski e admirando-o, aumenta consideravelmente o prazer desta leitura. Em “The Undoing Project” Lewis dá conta das principais teorias desenvolvidas e sua relevância, mas é no livro de Kahneman que podem encontrar uma porta segura para se iniciarem. De forma muito resumida, Kahneman e Tverski são responsáveis por uma mudança de 180º na forma como passámos a encarar os seres humanos, de seres racionais a seres emocionais, nomeadamente em tudo o que tem que ver com o modo como se processa a tomada de decisões. Até ao surgimento do trabalho desta dupla, os modelos dos economistas criavam previsões partindo do princípio de que os seres humanos eram profundamente racionais, que agiam baseados em conceitos probabilísticos, capazes de quantificar os ganhos e as perdas, e tomar decisões lógicas nas suas vidas. Kahneman e Tverski demonstraram que os seres humanos são tudo menos isso, que a racionalidade não está nunca separada da emocionalidade, e que existe um conjunto de processos que toldam e enviesam o modo como vemos e compreendemos o mundo. Lewis ao longo do livro vai usar toda a psicologia da dupla para nos dar conta da história de amizade que os levou a manterem-se juntos por mais de uma década, e depois novamente na hora da morte de um deles. Lewis podia ter-se focado sobre os processos de criação em duo, algo que sabemos bem ser imensamente complexo, contudo acabou por se concentrar mais sobre a amizade entre ambos, sobre o modo como se entendiam e aceitavam, sobre o como os opostos se atraem. Lewis cria uma quase história de amor, carregada de poética, beleza e sonho, capaz de produzir uma intensa carga inspiracional em quem lê. Não poderia recomendar mais. Publicado no Virtual Illusion (https://virtual-illusion.blogspot.pt/...)

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven
4 stars
Mar 20, 2022

The book describes the relationship between Amos and Danny, two Israeli psychologists that have changed all our lives without us knowing. After finishing the book I spent a whole weekend researching and watching youtube videos. At one point I caught myself contemplating studying behavioral psychology. Thank god tomorrow is Monday 😉

+4
Photo of Shreerag Plakazhi
Shreerag Plakazhi@shreerag
5 stars
Sep 8, 2021

I loved this book so much. This is the first Micheal Lewis book I'm reading, and it will not be the last. He manages to captures the essence of three people - Amos, Danny, and the conjoined Amos and Danny, who really are one mind in two bodies when they work. He captures so well their early lives, how they fit into each other, and how they fell apart. On top of all this he provides us with a fantastic primer on their incredible work. Prospect theory is foundational for so much research, as is much of the work they did really. I am inspired into action! Highly recommended!

Photo of Ilia Markov
Ilia Markov@ilia
4 stars
Aug 1, 2021

Hard to follow when listening to, but a great book about some of the best scientists of our age

Photo of Jose Quaresma
Jose Quaresma@josequaresma
5 stars
Jul 28, 2021

Wonderful book on the story behind Kahneman and Tversky's special partnership. To be paired with "Thinking, Fast and Slow"

Photo of Andrew John Kinney
Andrew John Kinney@numidica
5 stars
Aug 18, 2023
Photo of Said Afandi
Said Afandi@SaidAfandi
4 stars
Dec 30, 2022
+6
Photo of Hemanth Soni
Hemanth Soni@hemaaanth
3 stars
Aug 12, 2022
Photo of David Furnes
David Furnes@dfurnes
4 stars
Jan 25, 2024
Photo of Brock
Brock@brock
4 stars
Jan 3, 2024
Photo of rob suttman
rob suttman@stigs_cousin
4 stars
Dec 26, 2023
Photo of Jayme Cochrane
Jayme Cochrane@jamesco
4 stars
Dec 20, 2023
Photo of Heiki Riesenkampf
Heiki Riesenkampf@hrk
3 stars
Dec 18, 2023
Photo of Stefan Ladstätter-Thaa
Stefan Ladstätter-Thaa@stefan786
4 stars
Oct 23, 2023
Photo of Gabriel Ayuso
Gabriel Ayuso@gabrielayuso
4 stars
Jun 30, 2023
Photo of Erin Lee
Erin Lee@itserinleeslibrary
5 stars
Mar 25, 2023
Photo of Sujay Kathrotia
Sujay Kathrotia@sujay
3 stars
Mar 20, 2023
Photo of Róbert Istók
Róbert Istók@robertistok
5 stars
Mar 19, 2023
Photo of Mac Navarro
Mac Navarro@1xmac
5 stars
Mar 1, 2023
Photo of Bouke van der Bijl
Bouke van der Bijl@bouk
3 stars
Mar 1, 2023

Highlights

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

But because he was Danny, he made a rule about his fantasy life: He never fantasized about something that might happen. He established this private rule for his imagination once he realized that, after he had fantasized about something that might actually happen, he lost his drive to make it happen. His fantasies were so vivid that “it was as if you actually had it,” and if you actually had it, why would you bother to work hard to get it?

Page 302
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

The homeless were a notorious drag on the local health care system. They turned up in emergency rooms more often than they needed to. They were a drain on resources. Every nurse in Toronto knew this: If you see a homeless person wander in, hustle him out the door as fast as you can. Redelmeier wondered about the wisdom of that strategy. And so, in 1991, he created an experiment. He arranged for large numbers of college students who wanted to become doctors to be given hospital greens and a place to sleep near the emergency room. Their job was to serve as concierges to the homeless. When a homeless person entered the emergency room, they were to tend to his every need. Fetch him juice and a sandwich, sit down and talk to him, help arrange for his medical care. The college students worked for free. They loved it: They got to pretend to be doctors. But they serviced only half of the homeless people who entered the hospital. The other half received the usual curt and dismissive service from the nursing staff. Redelmeier then tracked the subsequent use of the Toronto health care system by all the homeless people who had visited his hospital. Unsurprisingly, the group that received the gold-plated concierge service tended to return slightly more often to the hospital where they had received it than the unlucky group. The surprise was that their use of the greater Toronto health care system declined. When homeless people felt taken care of by a hospital, they didn’t look for other hospitals that might take care of them. The homeless said, “That was the best that can be done for me.” The entire Toronto health care system had been paying a price for its attitude to the homeless.

Page 296

Such a great story. Trying to transfer this to other situations...

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

They explained that people chose the more detailed description, even though it was less probable, because it was more “representative.” They pointed out some places in the real world where this kink in the mind might have serious consequences. Any prediction, for instance, could be made to seem more believable, even as it became less likely, if it was filled with internally consistent details. And any lawyer could at once make a case seem more persuasive, even as he made the truth of it less likely, by adding “representative” details to his description of people and events.

Page 282

...again the Linda problem aka. Conjunction fallacy

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People were blind to logic when it was embedded in a story. Describe a very sick old man and ask people: Which is more probable, that he will die within a week or die within a year? More often than not, they will say, “He’ll die within a week.” Their mind latches onto a story of imminent death and the story masks the logic of the situation.

Page 280

The Linda problem aka. Conjunction fallacy

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

The present world is often surprising, i.e., less plausible than some of its alternatives, [...] the context of alternatives or the possibility set determines our expectations, our interpretations, our recollection and our attribution of reality, as well as the affective states which it induces.“ Toward the end of his thinking on the subject, [Amos] summed up a lot in a single sentence: “Reality is a cloud of possibility, not a point.”

Page 170
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

[...] the rules of undoing. One rule was that the more items there were to undo in order to create some alternative reality, the less likely the mind was to undo them. People seemed less likely to undo someone being killed by a massive earthquake than they were to undo a person’s being killed by a bolt of lightning, because undoing the earthquake required them to undo all the earthquake had done. “The more consequences an event has, the larger the change that is involved in eliminating that event,” Danny wrote to Amos. Another, related, rule was that “an event becomes gradually less changeable as it recedes into the past.” With the passage of time, the consequences of any event accumulated, and left more to undo. And the more there is to undo, the less likely the mind is to even try. This was perhaps one way time heals wounds, by making them feel less avoidable.

Page 262
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People did not choose between things. They chose between descriptions of things. [...] Economists assumed that you could simply measure what people wanted from what they chose. But what if what you want changes with the context in which the options are offered to you? “It was a funny point to make because the point within psychology would have been banal,” the psychologist Richard Nisbett later said. “Of course we are affected by how the decision is presented!

Page 240
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

Problem A. In addition to whatever you own, you have been given $1,000. You are now required to choose between the following options: Option 1. A 50 percent chance to win $1000 Option 2. A gift of $500” Most everyone picked option 2, the sure thing. Problem B. In addition to whatever you own, you have been given $2,000. You are now required to choose between the following options: Option 3. A 50 percent chance to lose $1,000 Option 4. A sure loss of $500 Most everyone picked option 3, the gamble. The two questions were effectively identical. In both cases, if you picked the gamble, you wound up with a 50-50 shot at being worth $2,000. In both cases, if you picked the sure thing, you wound up being worth $1,500. But when you framed the sure thing as a loss, people chose the gamble. When you framed it as a gain, people picked the sure thing. The reference point—the point that enabled you to distinguish between a gain and a loss— wasn’t some fixed number. It was a psychological state.

Page 240

Framing

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

Whatever that emotion was, it became stronger as the odds became more remote. If you told them that there was a one-in-a-billion chance that they’d win or lose a bunch of money, they behaved as if the odds were not one in a billion but one in ten thousand. They feared a one-in-a-billion chance of loss more than they should and attached more hope to a one-in-a-billion chance of gain than they should. People’s emotional response to extremely long odds led them to reverse their usual taste for risk, and to become risk seeking when pursuing a long-shot gain and risk avoiding when faced with the extremely remote possibility of loss. (Which is why they bought both lottery tickets and insurance.)

Page 236

Baffling. But explains so much that doesn't make sense otherwise

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People regretted what they had done, and what they wished they hadn’t done, far more than what they had not done and perhaps should have. “The pain that is experienced when the loss is caused by an act that modified the status quo is significantly greater than the pain that is experienced when the decision led to the retention of the status quo,” Danny wrote in a memo to Amos. “When one fails to take action that could have avoided a disaster, one does not accept responsibility for the occurrence of the disaster.”

Page 228
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

Danny was stunned: If a 10 percent increase in the chances of full-scale war with Syria wasn’t enough to interest the director-general in Kissinger’s peace process, how much would it take to convince him? That number represented the best estimate of the odds. Apparently the director-general didn’t want to rely on the best estimates. He preferred his own internal probability calculator: his gut. “That was the moment I gave up on decision analysis,” said Danny. “No one ever made a decision because of a number. They need a story.” As Danny and Lanir wrote, decades later, after the U.S. Central Intelligence Agency asked them to describe their experience in decision analysis, the Israeli Foreign Ministry was “indifferent to the specific probabilities.” What was the point of laying out the odds of a gamble, if the person taking it either didn’t believe the numbers or didn’t want to know them? The trouble, Danny suspected, was that “the understanding of numbers is so weak that they don’t communicate anything. Everyone feels that those probabilities are not real—that they are just something on somebody’s mind.”

Page 218
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

It wasn’t just sports announcers and political pundits who radically revised their narratives, or shifted focus, so that their stories seemed to fit whatever had just happened in a game or an election. Historians imposed false order upon random events, too, probably without even realizing what they were doing. Amos had a phrase for this. “Creeping determinism,” he called it—and jotted in his notes one of its many costs: “He who sees the past as surprise-free is bound to have a future full of surprises.” A false view of what has happened in the past makes it harder to see what might occur in the future. The historians in his audience of course prided themselves on their “ability” to construct, out of fragments of some past reality, explanatory narratives of events which made them seem, in retrospect, almost predictable. The only question that remained, once the historian had explained how and why some event had occurred, was why the people in his narrative had not seen what the historian could now see.

Page 180
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

All too often, we find ourselves unable to predict what will happen; yet after the fact we explain what did happen with a great deal of confidence. This “ability” to explain that which we cannot predict, even in the absence of any additional information, represents an important, though subtle, flaw in our reasoning. It leads us to believe that there is a less uncertain world than there actually is, and that we are less bright than we actually might be. For if we can explain tomorrow what we cannot predict today, without any added information except the knowledge of the actual outcome, then this outcome must have been determined in advance and we should have been able to predict it. The fact that we couldn’t is taken as an indication of our limited intelligence rather than of the uncertainty that is in the world. All too often, we feel like kicking ourselves for failing to foresee that which later appears inevitable. For all we know, the handwriting might have been on the wall all along. The question is: was the ink visible?

Page 180

Case against determinism intensifies

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

After the trip, Fischhoff went back and asked the same people to recall the odds they had assigned to each outcome. Their memories of the odds they had assigned to various outcomes were badly distorted. They all believed that they had assigned higher probabilities to what happened than they actually had. They greatly overestimated the odds that they had assigned to what had actually happened. That is, once they knew the outcome, they thought it had been far more predictable than they had found it to be before, when they had tried to predict it. A few years after Amos described the work to his Buffalo audience, Fischhoff named the phenomenon “hindsight bias.”

Page 180
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People are very good at detecting patterns and trends even in random data. In contrast to our skill in inventing scenarios, explanations, and interpretations, our ability to assess their likelihood, or to evaluate them critically, is grossly inadequate. Once we have adopted a particular hypothesis or interpretation, we grossly exaggerate the likelihood of that hypothesis, and find it very difficult to see things any other way.

Page 178
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People predict by making up stories People predict very little and explain everything People live under uncertainty whether they like it or not People believe they can tell the future if they work hard enough People accept any explanation as long as it fits the facts The handwriting was on the wall, it was just the ink that was invisible People often work hard to obtain information they already have And avoid new knowledge Man is a deterministic device thrown into a probabilistic Universe In this match, surprises are expected Everything that has already happened must have been inevitable

Page 172

Somehow a case against determinism

Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

The psychologist Kurt Lewin had suggested persuasively that, rather than selling people on some change, you were better off identifying the reasons for their resistance, and addressing those. Imagine a plank held in place by a spring on either side of it, Danny told the students. How do you move it? Well, you can increase the force on one side of the plank. Or you can reduce the force on the other side. “In one case the overall tension is reduced,” he said, “and in the other it is increased.” And that was a sort of proof that there was an advantage in reducing the tension. “It’s a key idea,” said Danny. “Making it easy to change.”

Page 124
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

People said some strange things. For instance, they said that magenta was similar to red, but that red wasn’t similar to magenta. Amos spotted the contradiction and set out to generalize it. He asked people if they thought North Korea was like Red China. They said yes. He asked them if Red China was like North Korea—and they said no. People thought Tel Aviv was like New York but that New York was not like Tel Aviv. People thought that the number 103 was sort of like the number 100, but that 100 wasn’t like 103. People thought a toy train was a lot like a real train but that a real train was not like a toy train. People often thought that a son resembled his father, but if you asked them if the father resembled his son, they just looked at you strangely. [...] When people compared one thing to another—two people, two places, two numbers, two ideas—they did not pay much attention to symmetry. To Amos—and to no one else before Amos—it followed from this simple observation that all the theories that intellectuals had dreamed up to explain how people made similarity judgments had to be false.

Page 100
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

The human mind was just bad at seeing things it did not expect to see, and a bit too eager to see what it expected to see. “Confirmation bias is the most insidious because you don’t even realize it is happening,” he said. A scout would settle on an opinion about a player and then arrange the evidence to support that opinion.

Page 34
Photo of Sven Schmidt
Sven Schmidt@sven

From his stint as a consultant he learned something valuable, however. It seemed to him that a big part of a consultant’s job was to feign total certainty about uncertain things. In a job interview with McKinsey, they told him that he was not certain enough in his opinions. “And I said it was because I wasn’t certain. And they said, ‘We’re billing clients five hundred grand a year, so you have to be sure of what you are saying.’” The consulting firm that eventually hired him was forever asking him to exhibit confidence when, in his view, confidence was a sign of fraudulence. They’d asked him to forecast the price of oil for clients, for instance. “And then we would go to our clients and tell them we could predict the price of oil. No one can predict the price of oil. It was basically nonsense.

Page 24